How do you catch a cloud and pin it down?
A while back I finished reading How Children Learn at Home by Alan Thomas and Harriet Pattison. I really wanted to do a good job reviewing this book, because it started my mind going in all sorts of directions. I'm sure I made some stunning insights while in the shower. However, I needed to return the book to its rightful owner. And we've been very busy of late. So, yeah, this is as good as it's going to get. Unfortunately I am now reviewing the book without having it in front of me, so some of this is out of my dodgy memory.
The book is a result of research on 26 home schooling families (who followed an unstructured, informal approach to their children's learning), along with the application of educational theory and prior research. It explores an alternative way to become educated (informal learning at home with family).
Initially I read this book with great interest. I kept mentally nodding my head, thinking, yes! That makes so much sense. And when I went "yes, but...", often my question was answered in the next paragraph, which was very satisfying. The simplicity of the argument was compelling. If we know, through research, that free play at home is an effective way of learning for those 0-5, then why does this suddenly change when the child reaches "school age"? And if we can demonstrate that informally home schooled children can and do achieve success in life, then that in itself demonstrates that learning occurs and an education achieved.
Yes. But......
First, I was very interested in their interpretation of sociocultural theories of learning. Their comments (with respect to Vygotsky) that these theories were limited as they focused on how to teach rather than how children learned was genuinely illuminating. But at the same time I wondered if their reading of the material was correct. Certainly in the limited amount of reading I've done, I haven't thought of it that way before - it seemed to me that concepts like the Zone of Proximal Development could go both ways. I would have to do more reading though to be able to comment further on that. And I would love to read some work actually by Vygotsky (er...in translation!) rather than relying on the interpretations of others.
I had some problems too with the nature of their research. Somewhere in the book they say something like good parenting will result in an educated child. And it seemed like their 26 families were very good parents. Many were active in homeschooling communities, were dedicated and had a clear philosophy and an energy and passion for home education. So, not your average parent then! I wondered if the research had much general application. (It didn't necessarily need too, as that wasn't its focus. But I still wondered).
Because a lot of the research consisted of interviews with parents about their offspring - objectivity was nowhere in sight. We parents love to go on and on to anyone who will listen about how gorgeous, talented and generally excellent our precious babies are! (Hey, just explore this blog a little if you want an example!) Some of the anecdotes had a real "Gee whiz, he's so talented!" quality to them that grated a little. Some of the stories had...um...possible alternative explanations. For example, in one anecdote, a parent told proudly about how the son had read out his amazing story to a group of adults with total confidence. The impression given was that his powers of literacy and presentation wowed the crowd. Perhaps - just perhaps - they were shifting uncomfortably in their chairs wondering when this child was going to stop - please! (as the parent glowed with pride, blissfully unaware).
With respect to the research - I thought that a different method of gathering data could have been useful. At Playcentre, as is very common in early childhood education in New Zealand, we use learning stories to make informal learning visible. I'm proud of the portfolios that result - with tools such as a digital camera, a pen and enough time and love to just watch and notice, a wonderful record of learning can be created. This need not interfere with the learning process and can be either highly analytical, or just speak for itself. Would documenting learning in this way be an acceptable tracking process of informal learning, or would it be seen as too formal? I guess that would depend on the families' philosophies. It could be a very useful research tool. Blogs like this one can also provide incredibly rich information about the acquisition of informal learning. Sure, the filter of the parent is still there, but the children's learning is more open to a researcher's interpretations. I'm unconvinced that interviews give rich enough data to draw meaningful conclusions.
I was also struck by how often the parents said that children just knew stuff - were never taught. I wonder. I saw an example of this the other day - I can't really relate it online without identifying the person to at least three of my five readers, but I'll do my best to relate it. The child was demonstrating something that they had apparently spontaneously learnt. Yet watching, it was clear to me that I was watching a lesson. This was not a bad thing. In fact, it showed a parent doing a wonderful job doing what comes naturally. Yet because it did not involve a worksheet, a desk, or pen, paper or lecturing, it wasn't considered teaching. I guess my definition is a little wider.
A small curiousity from my perspective was the lack of attention to early childhood learning. It was almost as if there was the presumption that most children initially learn at home, then go to school (or at least learn informally in whatever ECE setting). In New Zealand at least most children attend something else (kindy, preschool, daycare, Playcentre etc), with the trend seemingly for more hours, earlier. Of course one may argue - as I would - that the informal learning these children experience at home is still of vital importance. Even a paragraph or two placing school-age learning in this larger educational context would have been helpful.
So, that was much of the first half of the book. I enjoyed it. It presented old ideas in a new fashion, and gave me some new ideas. All good stuff.
And on to the second half, which I found a lot more challenging. What I call the "second half" was more practical, looking at the acquisition of reading, writing, numeracy. I wasn't convinced. Despite the stories, no, I just don't believe that so long as there is literacy in the house, that children just teach themselves to read. Perhaps certain children in certain families. But for many, I think something more is necessary to break the code. And for me, no, it's not OK in the absence of special needs for a child to be not "ready" to read before they are 12. I can't imagine what I would have missed out on if that had been me.
Two other things I struggled with threaded through the second half. First, it seemed to be very important that children follow their interests. Fine, I can run with that, from both a practical and theoretical perspective. But it seemed such a sacred imperative - if a child was the least bit bored or unwilling, that was that. There was no obvious fostering of the learning dispositions of persevering through difficulties, or of applying yourself to do just what has to be done (even though it is boring or difficult). Sure, you can teach/experience that in other ways. (Housework comes to mind!). But again, I wonder. Even when doing things we love, sometimes there's boring or hard bits that you just have to push through to get to the (rewarding) other side.
(By the way, it would be usual at this point to say something like "this would not bode well in the world of work/"real" world. However, philosophically I believe that each life stage has value in and of itself rather than as a prelude to something else. So we can skip that bit).
The other thing was a strong thread of utilitarian learning - i.e. if it's not immediately useful (or the child cannot see the immediate use) it's not worth doing. I hear this often in statements like "well, how much of the maths you learnt at school do you actually use?" There was no analysis of educational process vs product that I can recall. It seemed highly ironic that learning for its own sake didn't appear to be valued in some of these unschooled contexts.
So, a worthwhile book and a thought-provoking read. I can't say that I'm a true believer though - if I were to homeschool I would require of myself a bit of structure (as of course some home educators do). And I'd like to see more research in this area so that we can know a bit more about this thing called informal learning.
The book is a result of research on 26 home schooling families (who followed an unstructured, informal approach to their children's learning), along with the application of educational theory and prior research. It explores an alternative way to become educated (informal learning at home with family).
Initially I read this book with great interest. I kept mentally nodding my head, thinking, yes! That makes so much sense. And when I went "yes, but...", often my question was answered in the next paragraph, which was very satisfying. The simplicity of the argument was compelling. If we know, through research, that free play at home is an effective way of learning for those 0-5, then why does this suddenly change when the child reaches "school age"? And if we can demonstrate that informally home schooled children can and do achieve success in life, then that in itself demonstrates that learning occurs and an education achieved.
Yes. But......
First, I was very interested in their interpretation of sociocultural theories of learning. Their comments (with respect to Vygotsky) that these theories were limited as they focused on how to teach rather than how children learned was genuinely illuminating. But at the same time I wondered if their reading of the material was correct. Certainly in the limited amount of reading I've done, I haven't thought of it that way before - it seemed to me that concepts like the Zone of Proximal Development could go both ways. I would have to do more reading though to be able to comment further on that. And I would love to read some work actually by Vygotsky (er...in translation!) rather than relying on the interpretations of others.
I had some problems too with the nature of their research. Somewhere in the book they say something like good parenting will result in an educated child. And it seemed like their 26 families were very good parents. Many were active in homeschooling communities, were dedicated and had a clear philosophy and an energy and passion for home education. So, not your average parent then! I wondered if the research had much general application. (It didn't necessarily need too, as that wasn't its focus. But I still wondered).
Because a lot of the research consisted of interviews with parents about their offspring - objectivity was nowhere in sight. We parents love to go on and on to anyone who will listen about how gorgeous, talented and generally excellent our precious babies are! (Hey, just explore this blog a little if you want an example!) Some of the anecdotes had a real "Gee whiz, he's so talented!" quality to them that grated a little. Some of the stories had...um...possible alternative explanations. For example, in one anecdote, a parent told proudly about how the son had read out his amazing story to a group of adults with total confidence. The impression given was that his powers of literacy and presentation wowed the crowd. Perhaps - just perhaps - they were shifting uncomfortably in their chairs wondering when this child was going to stop - please! (as the parent glowed with pride, blissfully unaware).
With respect to the research - I thought that a different method of gathering data could have been useful. At Playcentre, as is very common in early childhood education in New Zealand, we use learning stories to make informal learning visible. I'm proud of the portfolios that result - with tools such as a digital camera, a pen and enough time and love to just watch and notice, a wonderful record of learning can be created. This need not interfere with the learning process and can be either highly analytical, or just speak for itself. Would documenting learning in this way be an acceptable tracking process of informal learning, or would it be seen as too formal? I guess that would depend on the families' philosophies. It could be a very useful research tool. Blogs like this one can also provide incredibly rich information about the acquisition of informal learning. Sure, the filter of the parent is still there, but the children's learning is more open to a researcher's interpretations. I'm unconvinced that interviews give rich enough data to draw meaningful conclusions.
I was also struck by how often the parents said that children just knew stuff - were never taught. I wonder. I saw an example of this the other day - I can't really relate it online without identifying the person to at least three of my five readers, but I'll do my best to relate it. The child was demonstrating something that they had apparently spontaneously learnt. Yet watching, it was clear to me that I was watching a lesson. This was not a bad thing. In fact, it showed a parent doing a wonderful job doing what comes naturally. Yet because it did not involve a worksheet, a desk, or pen, paper or lecturing, it wasn't considered teaching. I guess my definition is a little wider.
A small curiousity from my perspective was the lack of attention to early childhood learning. It was almost as if there was the presumption that most children initially learn at home, then go to school (or at least learn informally in whatever ECE setting). In New Zealand at least most children attend something else (kindy, preschool, daycare, Playcentre etc), with the trend seemingly for more hours, earlier. Of course one may argue - as I would - that the informal learning these children experience at home is still of vital importance. Even a paragraph or two placing school-age learning in this larger educational context would have been helpful.
So, that was much of the first half of the book. I enjoyed it. It presented old ideas in a new fashion, and gave me some new ideas. All good stuff.
And on to the second half, which I found a lot more challenging. What I call the "second half" was more practical, looking at the acquisition of reading, writing, numeracy. I wasn't convinced. Despite the stories, no, I just don't believe that so long as there is literacy in the house, that children just teach themselves to read. Perhaps certain children in certain families. But for many, I think something more is necessary to break the code. And for me, no, it's not OK in the absence of special needs for a child to be not "ready" to read before they are 12. I can't imagine what I would have missed out on if that had been me.
Two other things I struggled with threaded through the second half. First, it seemed to be very important that children follow their interests. Fine, I can run with that, from both a practical and theoretical perspective. But it seemed such a sacred imperative - if a child was the least bit bored or unwilling, that was that. There was no obvious fostering of the learning dispositions of persevering through difficulties, or of applying yourself to do just what has to be done (even though it is boring or difficult). Sure, you can teach/experience that in other ways. (Housework comes to mind!). But again, I wonder. Even when doing things we love, sometimes there's boring or hard bits that you just have to push through to get to the (rewarding) other side.
(By the way, it would be usual at this point to say something like "this would not bode well in the world of work/"real" world. However, philosophically I believe that each life stage has value in and of itself rather than as a prelude to something else. So we can skip that bit).
The other thing was a strong thread of utilitarian learning - i.e. if it's not immediately useful (or the child cannot see the immediate use) it's not worth doing. I hear this often in statements like "well, how much of the maths you learnt at school do you actually use?" There was no analysis of educational process vs product that I can recall. It seemed highly ironic that learning for its own sake didn't appear to be valued in some of these unschooled contexts.
So, a worthwhile book and a thought-provoking read. I can't say that I'm a true believer though - if I were to homeschool I would require of myself a bit of structure (as of course some home educators do). And I'd like to see more research in this area so that we can know a bit more about this thing called informal learning.
Labels: education, Playcentre, reviews
7 Comments:
Thanks for the interesting review! Always good to get feedback from a non-true-believer for some perspective;-)
Very interesting.
You've got me thinking about the things I accidentally (but actively) teach at home. For example today I made a pathetic joke to D which relied on knowing that oxidation is the opposite of reduction then K asked me to explain what I said so I talked about apples going brown. Different families teach such different things.
what they said :)i've just finished reading it too..(well it had to go back before i could finish it!) the things that struck me were a) that i never *really* noticed before just how much the kids are learning constantly at home, without formal instruction.. and b)all the references to Magic; the Gathering cards & heavy computer use really got my ick factor up.
heh. if 'teaching' = pens & paper, desks & worksheets, then paul does very little teaching at all! ;) if fact a little while ago his class were making bread rolls, and once they had finished measuring & mixing everything & had popped it in the oven, one of the kids piped up in an accusing tone "hey! this is MATHS!!" lol... little did they know that it was also handwriting in the form of working on their gross & fine motor skills while pouring & kneading too ;)
I really appreciate this review Mary. A book I had intended to read last year and then forgot about. I would say more but for the two year old factor right now.
Hey Mary, that's a pretty thorough review given your initial disclaimers!
I have so many comments - I'd better try not to write a novel.
As an ex-playcentre unschooler, I have thought about using learning stories. I used to write quite formal, analytical ones. I just haven't seen much need now that I am unschooling. If somebody wanted to research my family, for a book, I agree learning stories would be a good tool. For my own assessment of my children, it seems unnecessary - I know where they are and I'm generally pleased with their progress.
I would say that mine is one unschooling house where learning is valued for its own sake and learning is persevered with even when it gets hard or boring. I do feel discomfort that some unschooling families have missed this idea and are too willing to quit or pronounce a subject "unnecessary" or "irrelevant". For me it is a matter of modelling a love of learning, and encouraging the children. The saying is "inspire don't require". Encouragement is necessary at times.
It is true that children just know things - one of mine jumped from 3 + 3 to 12 X 12 with zero input from me. It is surprising how often it happens.
Although I struggle with late reading too, late readers in a read-aloud family can be, paradoxically, highly literate in all but the mechanical sense - more literate than their "saddle club" obsessed schooled peers.
Can I recommend John Holt's "How Children Fail" as an interesting point of contrast? It's not about home education at all - but very interesting!
Oh, I just had to come back with a couple more comments on "late reading".
First, I don't think it's fair to compare a child reading at 12 (an extreme and unusual situation in home ed families) with yourself, who presumably read widely and with pleasure at an early age. A fairer comparison would be to compare the late home-educated reader with the late schooled reader, with all the attendant baggage and sense of failure that such a child experiences, often as early as six years of age.
Second, it seems to be a common misconception that unschooling requires parents to refuse to teach their children to read. Mine have all asked me to teach them. Of course I've always been "teaching" them to read, in the wider sense, but they've specifically asked for sustained and focused help in the mechanics of reading - two at the age of 5, the other at 7.
"The saying is "inspire don't require". Encouragement is necessary at times."
I love this Fire!
I still struggle with late reading - or perhaps not the late reading itself, more the relaxed approach? Certainly if I were to homeschool, I think that even I would need some kind of structure (and I'm pretty much a totally unroutined parent!). I'll have to think about this some more....
Post a Comment
<< Home